It’s funny to me that as many times as we hear the “live
like it was your last day” and “treat others as you would like to be treated”
lecture, we maybe live only a few number of days in our lives like this. I
decided that I wanted to complete the Day of Compassion, because I think I
could use a little more in my life, or a lot of more. If you were to ask me any
day before last week if compassion was one of my traits, I would tell you in a
heartbeat yes, but writing this blog today, I can tell you that I actually
found I dislike many of my usual daily actions because I don’t live life compassionate
enough. Completing this assignment, I realized quickly through my day that
compassion is not about being kind so you don’t hurt others feelings or being
that person who never does wrong, it is about doing things focused towards the
greater good of others in a manner which is understanding of how your actions
will impact those around you and your environment. To be quite honest, I had to
retrial my day of compassion, because I failed miserably at first. It is scary
to realize how self-concerned I am on a regular basis, even with just the
little things like getting angry at the dogs for playing to rough or being
upset at that baby crying in the store. I decided on my retrial, I needed to work
strongly on my empathy, which is the
understanding of another person’s views and feeling compassionate towards that
person because of this understanding (Davis et al., 2004). I used Davis and colleagues idea of empathy to really live my day as if I
was living it through the eyes of others around me.
I
began my day as usual, I woke up, ate breakfast, let the dogs out, made sure to
give them a good head pat before walking out of the door, and I headed off to
the shelter early that morning for my internship not only to get hours, but
with the idea in mind that I would act in prosocial
behaviors towards all of the dogs this morning. According to Brief and
colleagues, prosocial behaviors are the
action we engage in which are meant to benefit those around us. I completed the
enrichment toy/activity as usual, but instead of just throwing the item in their
run, I took the time to say hello to each dog individually, and when I could
not get out of a run because the dogs were blocking the door, I did not get
angry with the dogs, instead I tried to find a way to guide their attention
toward the other side of the run. This experience was both positive for the
dogs and for me. Then I went to school, and as I was riding my pirate bike to
class, I noticed a girl who looked as if they were in a rush, and her backpack
was slowing her down, so I pedaled back around towards her and asked if she
wanted my bike to get a faster ride since we were going downhill. I told her I
was not in a rush, and she seemed really grateful for my offer. I think this experience
provides evidence which support the empathy-altruism
hypothesis (Batson, 1991). The empathy-altruism
hypothesis states that when we see someone in need, feelings of concern
develop for them and we engage in actions which we believe will increase their
well-being. The opposite side of this hypothesis occurs when we do not believe
someone is in need, therefore, we worry about our own well-being. According to
Batson, the outcome that occurred for my situation was of satisfaction because
of the decrease in stress of the girl I assisted.
As
I continued my day, not much out of the ordinary occurred like it had this morning,
but I continued to keep on my mind thoughts of compassion, and viewing my world
from others eyes. Just talking on the phone to my mother, I told her I know
that I forget to tell her how much I love and miss her, and how much I
appreciate all they do for me, and even that small conversation made her day
that much better, which in turn, made me happy. As the day fell to night, I noticed
I felt so much better about myself and how my actions were impacting others, putting
me into almost a euphoric state, and being in this euphoric state was in turn
giving me the desire to continue to do good and help others. I was experiencing
the good mood effect, and loving it!
The good-mood effect is the idea
that being in a good mood will increase the amount of behaviors you do which
help others (Isen, 2008). Of course, the day came to an end, I realized that it
wasn’t easy to be live, breath, and think compassion every day, it is actually
hard work! But if I could implement even just half as much compassion into my
daily life as I did that day, I would be satisfied, and I plan to try to reach
this goal. Not only did I make a positive impact on others, but I made a positive
impact on myself as well, and you can’t argue with that. Anyone that I
disclosed this experiment too whom I had interacted with that day told me they
just thought I was in a really good mood, which means my personality doesn’t
stray too far from the actions I engaged in that day, but there was an apparent
difference. I will definitely be recommending this activity to friends and
family, as it really puts into perspective how compassion and empathy for
others can really bring out the best in everyone.
Until next blog,
Your Social Psychista
Word Count: 979
Davis, M. H., Soduerlund, T., Cole, J., Gadol, E.,
Kute, M., & Wiehing. J. (2004). Cognitions associated with attempts to
empathize: How do we imagine the perspective of another? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 1625 -1635.
Brief, A. P., & Motowidlo, S. J. (1986). Prosocial
organizational behaviors. The Academy of
Management Review, 11, (4): 710–725.
Batson, C. D. (1991). The altruism question. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Isen, A. M. (2008). Positive affect and decision
processese: Some recent theoretical developments with practical implications.
In C.P. Haugtvedt, P. M. Herr, & F. R. Kardes (Eds), Handbook of consumer psychology (pp. 273-296). NY: Taylor &
Francis.
No comments:
Post a Comment